The challenge, then, is not only to symbolically link one well-bounded form with one well-circumscribed conception (i.e., where those symbols are then words and the like), but to make sure that those pairings closely match the conventional system of symbols that is dominant in that communication environment (i.e., ensure that they are accepted as paradigmatic among participants).
A person’s language will be identified as pathological if it is unconventional in any of the following ways:
A person who uses this sort of unconventional language will be deemed to have failed to have met this challenge.
Imagine what might happen, then, if a person could not rely on a conventional ability to set boundaries, that is to say, to segment their information time-streams.
That is what Peters was trying to get at (and is what we will explore further), but she caromed off of the DKE into the realm of fantasy.
If you don’t know about signal-noise ratios, then go learn about them and come back… keeping in mind that sensation is information; really, what you should know about is sensory processes, perception, and so on, in regards to communication, which can be found on this page.
I have a suspicion (and nothing more at this point) that this tells us something about the high rate of incidence of people (a) who are on the autism spectrum (b) who are first identified as possibly being deaf or hard of hearing. Sometimes this happens due to the likes of infrequent eye contact, but also because some children tend not to respond to events such as loud noises behind them (and other amateur tests for deafness).
Notably, this skill can develop over time, where the person might start to use such variations on the long phrase as (a) individual word substitutions (to alter the intended meaning in context), or (b) paring off some of the material that a conventional partner might consider to be irrelevant to the meaning.
And if it’s not clear by now, then I haven’t been repeating myself enough… which doesn’t seem likely.